Skip to main content
Original Articles and Reviews

Modeling Evolutionary Changes in Information Transfer

Effects of Domestication on the Vocal Communication of Dogs (Canis familiaris)

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000300

Abstract. Interspecific communication provides good opportunity for studying signal evolution. In this theoretical paper, we hypothesized that vocal signaling in dogs may show specific changes that made it more suitable for interspecific communication in the anthropogenic niche. We assumed that (1) some dog vocalizations will diverge from the corresponding exemplars of wolves; (2) they provide comprehendible affective, indexical, and contextual information for humans; (3) some aspects of dog vocalizations are more typical for the interspecific than for the intraspecific domain. We found that the most unique type of vocalization in the dog is barking. We proved that human listeners can contextually categorize dog barks, as well as attribute distinct inner states of dogs based on the barks. We found that dogs are sensitive to both contextual and individual-specific features of other dogs’ barks. However, dogs showed almost no response to the bark emitted in isolation, which is one of the easiest to recognize by humans, indicating the possibility of a specific, new communicative role for barks, not present in its original function. Our conclusion is that the qualitative and quantitative proliferation of barks can be explained by mechanisms of evolution such as ritualization and adaptive radiation. Barks became suitable for conveying a more various set of information than the original barks of wolves did. Barks also became typical in such contexts where originally they were not used – such as the contact seeking calls of isolated specimens, apparently targeted at the human, and not at a canine audience.

References

  • Ackerly, D. D., Schwilk, D. W. & Webb, C. O. (2006). Niche evolution and adaptive radiation: Testing the order of trait divergence. Ecology, 87, S50–S61. doi: 10.1890/0012-9658(2006)87[50:NEAART]2.0.CO;2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Allen, K., Blascovich, J. & Mendes, W. B. (2002). Cardiovascular reactivity and the presence of pets, friends, and spouses: The truth about cats and dogs. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64, 727–739. doi: 10.1097/01.PSY.0000024236.11538.41 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Amiot, C. E. & Bastian, B. (2015). Towards a psychology of human-animal relations. Psychological Bulletin, 141, 6–47. doi: 10.1037/a0038147 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Andics, A., Gábor, A., Gácsi, M., Faragó, T., Szabó, D. & Miklósi, Á. (2016). Neural mechanisms for lexical processing in dogs. Science, 353, 1030–1032. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf3777 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Andics, A., Gácsi, M., Faragó, T., Kis, A. & Miklósi, Á. (2014). Voice-sensitive regions in the dog and human brain are revealed by comparative fMRI. Current Biology, 24, 574–578. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.01.058 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bálint, A., Faragó, T., Dóka, A., Miklósi, Á. & Pongrácz, P. (2013). ‘Beware, I am big and non-dangerous!’–Playfully growling dogs are perceived larger than their actual size by their canine audience. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 148, 128–137. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2013.07.013 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bálint, A., Faragó, T., Miklósi, Á. & Pongrácz, P. (2016). Threat level dependent manipulation of signaled body size – Dog growls’ indexical cues depend on the different levels of potential danger. Animal Cognition, 19, 1115–1131. doi: 10.1007/s10071-016-1019-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barker, S. B., Barker, R. T., Dawson, K. S. & Knisely, J. S. (1997). The use of the family life space diagram in establishing inter-connectedness: A preliminary study of sexual abuse survivors, their significant others, and pets. Individual Psychology: Journal of Adlerian Theory, Research & Practice, 53, 435–450. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Benton, M. J. (1996). Diversification and extinction in the history of life. Science, 268, 52–58. doi: 10.1126/science.7701342 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bentosela, M., Barrera, G., Jakovcevic, A., Elgier, A. M. & Mustaca, A. E. (2008). Effect of reinforcement, reinforcer omission and extinction on a communicative response in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). Behavioural Processes, 78, 464–469. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.03.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Blumstein, D. T. (1999). The evolution of functionally referential alarm communication: Multiple adaptations; multiple constraints. Evolution of Communication, 3, 135–147. doi: 10.1075/eoc.3.2.03blu First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bonas, S., McNicholas, J. & Collis, G. M. (2000). Pets in the network of family relationships: An empirical study. In A. L. PodberscekE. S. PaulJ. A. SerpellEds., Companion animals and us: Exploring the relationships between people and pets (pp. 209–236). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bradshaw, J. W. S. & Nott, H. M. R. (1995). Social and communication behaviour of companion dogs. In J. SerpellEd., The domestic dog – Its evolution, behaviour and interactions with people (pp. 115–130). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Brady, C. A. (1981). The vocal repertoire of the bush dog (Speothos venaticus), crab-eating fox (Cerdocyon thous), and maned wolf (Chrysocyon brachyurus). Animal Behaviour, 29, 649–669. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(81)80001-2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cohen, J. A. & Fox, M. W. (1976). Vocalizations in wild canids and possible effects of domestication. Behavioural Processes, 1, 77–92. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(76)90008-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Coppinger, R. & Coppinger, L. (2016). What is a dog?. Cicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Coppinger, R. & Feinstein, M. (1991). Hark! Hark! The dogs do bark…. A new theory on why dogs bark. Smithsonian, 21, 119–128. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Csányi, V. (2000). The “human behaviour complex” and the compulsion of communication: Key factors of human evolution. Semiotica, 128, 45–60. doi: 10.1515/semi.2000.128.3-4.243 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • D’Aniello, B., Alterisio, A., Scandurra, A., Petremolo, E., Iommelli, M. R. & Aria, M. (2017). What’s the point? Golden and Labrador retrievers living in kennels do not understand human pointing gestures. Animal Cognition, 20. doi: 10.1007/s10071-017-1098-2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • D’Aniello, B. & Scandurra, A. (2016). Ontogenetic effects on gazing behaviour: A case study of kennel dogs (Labrador Retrievers) in the impossible task paradigm. Animal Cognition, 19, 565–570. doi: 10.1007/s10071-016-0958-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Déaux, É. C. & Clarke, J. A. (2013). Dingo (Canis lupus dingo) acoustic repertoire: Form and contexts. Behaviour, 150, 75–101. doi: 10.1163/1568539X-00003038 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Diamond, J. M. (1986). Evolution of ecological segregation in the New Guinea montane avifauna. In J. M. DiamondT. J. CaseEds., Community ecology (pp. S98–S125). Cambridge, MA: Harper & Row. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Faragó, T., Andics, A., Devecseri, V., Kis, A., Gácsi, M. & Miklósi, Á. (2014). Humans rely on the same rules to assess emotional valence and intensity in conspecific and dog vocalizations. Biology Letters, 10, 20130926. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2013.0926 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Faragó, T., Pongrácz, P., Miklósi, Á., Huber, L., Virányi, Zs. & Range, F. (2010a). Dogs’ expectation about signalers’ body size by virtue of their growls. PLoS One, 5, e15175. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015175 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Faragó, T., Pongrácz, P., Range, F., Virányi, Zs. & Miklósi, Á. (2010b). ‘The bone is mine’: Affective and referential aspects of dog growls. Animal Behaviour, 79, 917–925. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.01.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Faragó, T., Takács, N., Miklósi, Á. & Pongrácz, P. (2017). Dog growls express various contextual and affective content for human listeners. Royal Society Open Science, 4, 170134. doi: 10.1098/rsos.170134 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Feddersen-Petersen, D. (1991). The ontogeny of social play and agonistic behaviour in selected canid species. Bonner Zoologische Beiträge, 42, 97–114. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Feddersen-Petersen, D. U. (2000). Vocalisation of European wolves (Canis lupus lupus L.) and various dog breeds (Canis lupus f. familiaris). Archiv für Tierzucht, 43, 387–397. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fitch, W. T. & Hauser, M. D. (2003). Unpacking honesty: Vertebrate vocal production and the evolution of acoustic signals. In A. M. SimmonsA. N. PopperR. R. FayEds., Acoustic communication (pp. 65–137). New York, NY: Springer. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gácsi, M., Győri, B., Virányi, Z., Kubinyi, E., Range, F., Belényi, B. & Miklósi, Á. (2009). Explaining dog wolf differences in utilizing human pointing gestures: Selection for synergistic shifts in the development of some social skills. PLoS One, 4, e6584. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006584 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gácsi, M., Vas, J., Topál, J. & Miklósi, Á. (2013). Wolves do not join the dance: Sophisticated aggression control by adjusting to human social signals in dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 145, 109–122. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2013.02.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gosling, S. D., Kwan, V. S. & John, O. P. (2003). A dog’s got personality: A cross-species comparative approach to personality judgments in dogs and humans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1161–1169. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.1161 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hare, B., Brown, M., Williamson, C. & Tomasello, M. (2002). The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science, 298, 1634–1636. doi: 10.1126/science.1072702 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hart, B. L. & Miller, M. F. (1985). Behavioral profiles of dog breeds. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association, 186, 1175–1180. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hinde, R. A. (1981). Animal signals: Ethological and games-theory approaches are not incompatible. Animal Behaviour, 29, 535–542. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(81)80116-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hostetter, A. B. (2011). When do gestures communicate? A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 297–315. doi: 10.1037/a0022128 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Isack, H. A. & Reyer, H. U. (1989). Honeyguides and honey gatherers: Interspecific communication in a symbiotic relationship. Science, 243, 1343–1346. doi: 10.1126/science.243.4896.1343 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kostan, K. M. (2002). The evolution of mutualistic interspecific communication: assessment and management across species. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 116, 206–209. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.116.2.206 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kwan, S. Y. V., Gosling, S. D. & John, O. P. (2008). Anthropomorphism as a special case of social perception: A cross-species social relations model analysis of humans and dogs. Social Cognition, 26, 129–142. doi: 10.1521/soco.2008.26.2.129 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Larrañaga, A., Bielza, C., Pongrácz, P., Faragó, T., Bálint, A. & Larrañaga, P. (2015). Comparing supervised learning methods for classifying sex, age, context and individual Mudi dogs from barking. Animal Cognition, 18, 405–421. doi: 10.1007/s10071-014-0811-7 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lehner, P. N. (1978). Coyote vocalizations: A lexicon and comparisons with other canids. Animal Behaviour, 26, 712–722. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(78)90138-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • LoBue, V., Bloom Pickard, M., Sherman, K., Axford, C. & DeLoache, J. S. (2013). Young children’s interest in live animals. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31, 57–69. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2012.02078.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lord, K., Feinstein, M. & Coppinger, R. (2009). Barking and mobbing. Behavioural Processes, 81, 358–368. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2009.04.008 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Marler, P., Evans, C. S. & Hauser, M. D. (1992). Animal signals: Motivational, referential, or both. In H. PapousekU. JürgensM. PapousekEds., Nonverbal vocal communication: Comparative and developmental approaches (pp. 66–86). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Maros, K., Pongrácz, P., Bárdos, Gy., Molnár, Cs., Faragó, T. & Miklósi, Á. (2008). Dogs can discriminate barks from different situations. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 114, 159–167. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2008.01.022 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Marshall, D. A. & Moulton, D. G. (1981). Olfactory sensitivity to α-ionone in humans and dogs. Chemical Senses, 6, 53–61. doi: 10.1093/chemse/6.1.53 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McCarley, H. (1978). Vocalisations of red wolves (Canis rufus). Journal of Mammalogy, 59, 27–35. doi: 10.2307/1379872 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McComb, K., Taylor, A. M., Wilson, C. & Charlton, B. D. (2009). The cry embedded within the purr. Current Biology, 19, R507–R508. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.05.033 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McConnell, A. R., Brown, C. M., Shoda, T. M., Stayton, L. E. & Martin, C. E. (2011). Friends with benefits: On the positive consequences of pet ownership. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 1239–1252. doi: 10.1037/a0024506 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Meints, K., Racca, A. & Hickey, N. (2010). Child-dog misunderstandings: Children misinterpret dogs’ facial expressions. Proceedings of the 2nd Canine Science Forum (p. 99). Austria, Vienna. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Miklósi, Á., Kubinyi, E., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Virányi, Z. & Csányi, V. (2003). A simple reason for a big difference: Wolves do not look back at humans, but dogs do. Current Biology, 13, 763–766. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00263-X First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Miklósi, Á., Pongrácz, P., Lakatos, G., Topál, J. & Csányi, V. (2005). A comparative study of the use of visual communicative signals in interactions between dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans and cats (Felis catus) and humans. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119, 179–186. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.119.2.179 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Miklósi, Á. & Topál, J. (2013). What does it take to become ‘best friends’? Evolutionary changes in canine social competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 287–294. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.04.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Molnár, Cs., Kaplan, F., Roy, P., Pachet, F., Pongrácz, P., Dóka, A. & Miklósi, Á. (2008). Classification of dog barks: A machine learning approach. Animal Cognition, 11, 389–400. doi: 10.1007/s10071-007-0129-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Molnár, Cs., Pongrácz, P., Dóka, A. & Miklósi, Á. (2006). Can humans discriminate between dogs on the base of the acoustic parameters of barks? Behavioural Processes, 73, 76–83. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2006.03.014 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Molnár, Cs., Pongrácz, P., Faragó, T., Dóka, A. & Miklósi, Á. (2009). Dogs discriminate between barks: The effect of context and identity of the caller. Behavioural Processes, 82, 198–201. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2009.06.011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Molnár, Cs., Pongrácz, P. & Miklósi, Á. (2010). Seeing with ears: Sightless humans’ perception of dog bark provides a test for structural rules in vocal communication. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1004–1013. doi: 10.1080/17470210903168243 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Morton, E. S. (1977). On the occurrence and significance of motivation-structural rules in some bird and mammal sounds. American Naturalist, 111, 855–869. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Morton, E. S. (1994). Sound symbolism and its role in non-human vertebrate communication. In L. HintonJ. NicholsJ. J. OhalaEds., Sound symbolism (pp. 348–365). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Newton-Fisher, N., Harris, S., White, P. & Jones, G. (1993). Structure and function of red fox (Vulpes vulpes) vocalisations. Bioacoustics, 5, 1–31. doi: 10.1080/09524622.1993.9753228 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nicastro, N. & Owren, M. J. (2003). Classification of domestic cat (Felis catus) vocalizations by naive and experienced human listeners. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117, 44–52. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.117.1.44 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Odendaal, J. S. J. & Meintjes, R. A. (2003). Neurophysiological correlates of affiliative behaviour between humans and dogs. Veterinary Journal, 165, 296–301. doi: 10.1016/S1090-0233(02)00237-X First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Otte D.Endler J. A. (Eds.) (1989). Speciation and its consequences. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Association. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Polgárdi, R., Topál, J. & Csányi, V. (2000). Intentional behaviour in dog-human communication: An experimental analysis of “showing” behaviour in the dog. Animal Cognition, 3, 159–166. doi: 10.1007/s100710000072 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pongrácz, P., Czinege, N., Haynes, T. M. P., Tokumaru, R. S., Miklósi, Á. & Faragó, T. (2016). The communicative relevance of auditory nuisance – Barks that are connected to negative inner states in dogs can predict annoyance level in humans. Interaction Studies, 17, 19–40. doi: 10.1075/is.17.1.02pon First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pongrácz, P., Miklósi, Á., Timár-Geng, K. & Csányi, V. (2004). Verbal attention getting as a key factor in social learning between dog (Canis familiaris) and human. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 375–383. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.118.4.375 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pongrácz, P., Miklósi, Á., Molnár, Cs. & Csányi, V. (2005). Human listeners are able to classify dog barks recorded in different situations. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119, 136–144. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.119.2.136 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pongrácz, P., Molnár, Cs. & Miklósi, Á. (2006). Acoustic parameters of dog barks carry emotional information for humans. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 100, 228–240. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.12.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pongrácz, P., Molnár, Cs. & Miklósi, Á. (2010). Barking in family dogs: An ethological approach. The Veterinary Journal, 183, 141–147. doi: 10.1016/j.tvjl.2008.12.010 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pongrácz, P., Molnár, Cs., Dóka, A. & Miklósi, Á. (2011). Do children understand man’s best friend? Classification of dog barks by pre-adolescents and adults. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 135, 95–102. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2011.09.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pongrácz, P., Szabó, É., Kis, A., Péter, A. & Miklósi, Á. (2014). More than noise? Field investigations of intraspecific acoustic communication in dogs (Canis familiaris). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 159, 62–68. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2014.08.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Price, E. O. (1999). Behavioral development in animals undergoing domestication. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 65, 245–271. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(99)00087-8 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Riede, T. & Fitch, W. T. (1999). Vocal tract length and acoustics of vocalization in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Journal of Experimental Biology, 202, 2859–2867. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Robbins, R. L. (2000). Vocal communication in free-ranging African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus). Behaviour, 137, 1271–1298. doi: 10.1163/156853900501926 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schassburger, R. M. (1987). Wolf vocalization: An integrated model of structure, motivation and ontogeny. In H. FrankEd., Man and Wolf: Advances, issues, and problems in captive wolf research (pp. 313–347). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Dr. V. Junk Publishers. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schassburger, R. M. (1993). Vocal communication in the timber wolf, Canis lupus, Linnaeus: Structure, motivation, and ontogeny (Vol. 30, Advances in Ethology). Berlin, Germany: Paul Parey Scientific. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Scott-Phillips, T. C. (2008). Defining biological communication. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 21, 387–395. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2007.01497.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sevillano, V. & Fiske, S. T. (2016). Animals as social objects: Groups, stereotypes, and intergroup threats. European Psychologist, 21, 206–217. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000268 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Sugawara, A., Masud, M. M., Yokoyama, A., Mizutani, W., Watanuki, S., Yanai, K., … Tashiro, M. (2012). Effects of presence of a familiar pet dog on regional cerebral activity in healthy volunteers: A positron emission tomography study. Anthrozoös, 25, 25–34. doi: 10.2752/175303712X13240472427311 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Swain, J. E., Tasgin, E., Mayes, L. C., Feldman, R., Todd Constable, R. & Leckman, J. F. (2008). Maternal brain response to own baby‐cry is affected by cesarean section delivery. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 1042–1052. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01963.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Taylor, A. M., Ratcliffe, V. F., McComb, K. & Reby, D. (2014). Auditory communication in domestic dogs: Vocal signalling in an extended social environment of a companion animal. In J. KaminskiS. Marshall-PesciniEds., The social dog – Behaviour and cognition (pp. 131–164). Oxford, UK: Elsevier. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Taylor, A. M., Reby, D. & McComb, K. (2008). Human listeners attend to size information in domestic dog growls. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 123, 2903–2909. doi: 10.1121/1.2896962 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Taylor, A. M., Reby, D. & McComb, K. (2009). Context‐related variation in the vocal growling behaviour of the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Ethology, 115, 905–915. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01681.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Taylor, A. M., Reby, D. & McComb, K. (2010). Size communication in domestic dog, Canis familiaris, growls. Animal Behaviour, 79, 205–210. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.10.030 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tembrock, G. (1976). Canid vocalisations. Behavioural Processes, 1, 57–75. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(76)90007-3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Miklósi, Á., Virányi, Z., Kubinyi, E. & Csányi, V. (2005). Attachment to humans: A comparative study on hand-reared wolves and differently socialized dog puppies. Animal Behaviour, 70, 1367–1375. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.03.025 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Topál, J., Gergely, G., Erdőhegyi, Á., Csibra, G. & Miklósi, Á. (2009a). Differential sensitivity to human communication in dogs, wolves, and human infants. Science, 325, 1269–1272. doi: 10.1126/science.1176960 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., Gácsi, M., Dóka, A., Pongrácz, P., Kubinyi, E., … Csányi, V. (2009b). The dog as a model for understanding human social behavior. Advances in the Study of Animal Behaviour, 39, 71–116. doi: 10.1016/S0065-3454(09)39003-8 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Turcsán, B., Range, F., Virányi, Z., Miklósi, Á. & Kubinyi, E. (2012). Birds of a feather flock together? Perceived personality matching in owner–dog dyads. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 140, 154–160. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2012.06.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Udell, M. A. R. & Wynne, C. D. L. (2008). Review of domestic dogs’ (Canis familiaris) human-like behaviors: Or why behavior analysts should stop worrying and love their dogs. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 89, 247–261. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2008.89-247 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vas, J., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Miklósi, A. & Csányi, V. (2005). A friend or an enemy? Dogs’ reaction to an unfamiliar person showing behavioural cues of threat and friendliness at different times. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 94, 99–115. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.02.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Virányi, Z., Gácsi, M., Kubinyi, E., Topál, J., Belényi, B., Ujfalussy, D. & Miklósi, Á. (2008). Comprehension of human pointing gestures in young human-reared wolves (Canis lupus) and dogs (Canis familiaris). Animal Cognition, 11, 373. doi: 10.1007/s10071-007-0127-y First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Volodin, L. A., Volodina, E. V. & Isaeva, I. V. (2001). Vocal repertoire in the dhole Cuon alpinus (Carnivora, Canidae) in captivity. Entomological Review, 81, 161–166. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wheatcroft, D. & Price, T. D. (2015). Rates of signal evolution are associated with the nature of interspecific communication. Behavioral Ecology, 26, 83–90. doi: 10.1093/beheco/aru161 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wiley, R. H. (1983). The evolution of communication: Information and manipulation. Animal Behaviour, 2, 156–189. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wiley, R. H. (2006). Signal detection and animal communication. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 36, 217–247. doi: 10.1016/S0065-3454(06)36005-6 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wilson, E. O. (1993). Biophilia and the conservation ethics. In S. R. KellertE. O. WilsonEds., The biophilia hypothesis (pp. 31–41). Washington, DC: Island Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Yin, S. (2002). A new perspective on barking in dogs (Canis familiaris). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 116, 189–193. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.116.2.189 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yin, S. & McCowan, B. (2004). Barking in domestic dogs: context specificity and individual identification. Animal Behaviour, 68, 343–355. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.07.016 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar